Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
1.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 70(3): 565-578, 2017 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27028661

RESUMEN

Individuals with schizophrenia typically show increased levels of distractibility. This has been attributed to impaired working memory capacity (WMC), since lower WMC is typically associated with higher distractibility, and schizophrenia is typically associated with impoverished WMC. Here, participants performed verbal and spatial serial recall tasks that were accompanied by to-be-ignored speech tokens. For the few trials wherein one speech token was replaced with a different token, impairment was produced to task scores (a deviation effect). Participants subsequently completed a schizotypy questionnaire and a WMC measure. Higher schizotypy scores were associated with lower WMC (as measured with operation span, OSPAN), but WMC and schizotypy scores explained unique variance in relation to the mean magnitude of the deviation effect. These results suggest that schizotypy is associated with heightened domain-general distractibility, but that this is independent of its relationship with WMC.


Asunto(s)
Individualidad , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Trastorno de la Personalidad Esquizotípica/complicaciones , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Recuerdo Mental , Estimulación Luminosa , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Aprendizaje Verbal , Adulto Joven
2.
Int J Audiol ; 55(11): 623-42, 2016 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27589015

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The aims of the current n200 study were to assess the structural relations between three classes of test variables (i.e. HEARING, COGNITION and aided speech-in-noise OUTCOMES) and to describe the theoretical implications of these relations for the Ease of Language Understanding (ELU) model. STUDY SAMPLE: Participants were 200 hard-of-hearing hearing-aid users, with a mean age of 60.8 years. Forty-three percent were females and the mean hearing threshold in the better ear was 37.4 dB HL. DESIGN: LEVEL1 factor analyses extracted one factor per test and/or cognitive function based on a priori conceptualizations. The more abstract LEVEL 2 factor analyses were performed separately for the three classes of test variables. RESULTS: The HEARING test variables resulted in two LEVEL 2 factors, which we labelled SENSITIVITY and TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE; the COGNITIVE variables in one COGNITION factor only, and OUTCOMES in two factors, NO CONTEXT and CONTEXT. COGNITION predicted the NO CONTEXT factor to a stronger extent than the CONTEXT outcome factor. TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE and SENSITIVITY were associated with COGNITION and all three contributed significantly and independently to especially the NO CONTEXT outcome scores (R(2) = 0.40). CONCLUSIONS: All LEVEL 2 factors are important theoretically as well as for clinical assessment.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Corrección de Deficiencia Auditiva/instrumentación , Corrección de Deficiencia Auditiva/psicología , Audífonos , Trastornos de la Audición/psicología , Trastornos de la Audición/terapia , Personas con Deficiencia Auditiva/psicología , Personas con Deficiencia Auditiva/rehabilitación , Inteligibilidad del Habla , Percepción del Habla , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Audiometría de Tonos Puros , Umbral Auditivo , Comprensión , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Audición , Trastornos de la Audición/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Audición/fisiopatología , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Ruido/efectos adversos , Enmascaramiento Perceptual
3.
Scand J Psychol ; 56(1): 1-10, 2015 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25352319

RESUMEN

The present study used fMRI/BOLD neuroimaging to investigate how visual-verbal working memory is updated when exposed to three different background-noise conditions: speech noise, aircraft noise and silence. The number-updating task that was used can distinguish between "substitution processes," which involve adding new items to the working memory representation and suppressing old items, and "exclusion processes," which involve rejecting new items and maintaining an intact memory set. The current findings supported the findings of a previous study by showing that substitution activated the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the posterior medial frontal cortex and the parietal lobes, whereas exclusion activated the anterior medial frontal cortex. Moreover, the prefrontal cortex was activated more by substitution processes when exposed to background speech than when exposed to aircraft noise. These results indicate that (a) the prefrontal cortex plays a special role when task-irrelevant materials should be denied access to working memory and (b) that, when compensating for different types of noise, either different cognitive mechanisms are involved or those cognitive mechanisms that are involved are involved to different degrees.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Ruido , Habla , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adulto Joven
4.
PLoS One ; 8(12): e80719, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24324623

RESUMEN

Participants tasted two cups of coffee, decided which they preferred, and then rated each coffee. They were told (in lure) that one of the cups contained "eco-friendly" coffee while the other did not, although the two cups contained identical coffee. In Experiments 1 and 3, but not in Experiment 2, the participants were also told which cup contained which type of coffee before they tasted. The participants preferred the taste of, and were willing to pay more for, the "eco-friendly" coffee, at least those who scored high on a questionnaire on attitudes toward sustainable consumer behavior (Experiment 1). High sustainability consumers were also willing to pay more for "eco-friendly" coffee, even when they were told, after their decision, that they preferred the non-labeled alternative (Experiment 2). Moreover, the eco-label effect does not appear to be a consequence of social desirability, as participants were just as biased when reporting the taste estimates and willingness to pay anonymously (Experiment 3). Eco labels not only promote a willingness to pay more for the product but also lead to a more favorable perceptual experience of it.


Asunto(s)
Café , Etiquetado de Alimentos , Preferencias Alimentarias/psicología , Percepción del Gusto/fisiología , Gusto/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Alimentos Orgánicos/economía , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
5.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 24(11): 2147-54, 2012 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22849400

RESUMEN

Two fundamental research questions have driven attention research in the past: One concerns whether selection of relevant information among competing, irrelevant, information takes place at an early or at a late processing stage; the other concerns whether the capacity of attention is limited by a central, domain-general pool of resources or by independent, modality-specific pools. In this article, we contribute to these debates by showing that the auditory-evoked brainstem response (an early stage of auditory processing) to task-irrelevant sound decreases as a function of central working memory load (manipulated with a visual-verbal version of the n-back task). Furthermore, individual differences in central/domain-general working memory capacity modulated the magnitude of the auditory-evoked brainstem response, but only in the high working memory load condition. The results support a unified view of attention whereby the capacity of a late/central mechanism (working memory) modulates early precortical sensory processing.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Tronco Encefálico/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Filtrado Sensorial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
6.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 19(2): 245-50, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22231726

RESUMEN

Habituation of the orienting response is a pivotal part of selective attention, and previous research has related working memory capacity (WMC) to attention control. Against this background, the purpose of this study was to investigate whether individual differences in WMC contribute to habituation rate. The participants categorized visual targets across six blocks of trials. Each target was preceded either by a standard sound or, on rare trials, by a deviant. The magnitude of the deviation effect (i.e., prolonged response time when the deviant was presented) was relatively large in the beginning but attenuated toward the end. There was no relationship between WMC and the deviation effect at the beginning, but there was at the end, and greater WMC was associated with greater habituation. These results indicate that high memory ability increases habituation rate, and they support theories proposing a role for cognitive control in habituation and in some forms of auditory distraction.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Habituación Psicofisiológica , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Estimulación Acústica , Humanos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA